首页
登录
职称英语
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
游客
2023-12-15
26
管理
问题
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another case of experience changing the brain. From the sensory information we absorb to the movements we make, our lives leave footprints on the bumps and fissures of our cortex, so much so that experiences can alter "hard-wired" brain structures. Through rehab, stroke patients can coax a region of the motor cortex on the opposite side of the damaged region to pinch-hit, restoring lost mobility; volunteers who are blindfolded for just five days can reprogram their visual cortex to process sound and touch.
Still, scientists have been surprised at how deeply culture—the language we speak, the values we absorb—shapes the brain, and are rethinking findings derived from studies of Westerners. To take one recent example, a region behind the forehead called the medial prefrontal cortex supposedly represents the self: it is active when we ("we" being the Americans in the study) think of our own identity and traits. But with Chinese volunteers, the results were strikingly different. The "me" circuit hummed not only when they thought whether a particular adjective described themselves, but also when they considered whether it described their mother. The Westerners showed no such overlap between self and mom. Depending whether one lives in a culture that views the self as autonomous and unique or as connected to and part of a larger whole, this neural circuit takes on quite different functions.
"Cultural neuroscience," as this new field is called, is about discovering such differences. Some of the findings, as with the "me/mom" circuit, buttress longstanding notions of cultural differences. For instance, it is a cultural cliche that Westerners focus on individual objects while East Asians pay attention to context and background (another manifestation of the individualism-collectivism split). Sure enough, when shown complex, busy scenes, Asian-Americans and non-Asian—Americans recruited different brain regions. The Asians showed more activity in areas that process figure-ground relations—holistic context—while the Americans showed more activity in regions that recognize objects.
Psychologist Nalini Ambady of Tufts found something similar when she and colleagues showed drawings of people in a submissive pose (head down, shoulders hunched) or a dominant one (arms crossed, face forward) to Japanese and Americans. The brain’s dopamine-fueled reward circuit became most active at the sight of the stance—dominant for Americans, submissive for Japanese—that each volunteer’s culture most values, they reported in 2009. This raises an obvious chicken-and-egg question.
Cultural neuroscience wouldn’t be making waves if it found neurobiological bases only for well-known cultural differences. It is also uncovering the unexpected. For instance, a 2006 study found that native Chinese speakers use a different region of the brain to do simple arithmetic (3 + 4) or decide which number is larger than native English speakers do, even though both use Arabic numerals. The Chinese use the circuits that process visual and spatial information and plan movements (the latter may be related to the use of the abacus). But English speakers use language circuits. It is as if the West conceives numbers as just words, but the East imbues them with symbolic, spatial freight. "One would think that neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal," says Ambady, but they "seem to be culture-specific. "
Not to be the skunk at this party, but I think it’s important to ask whether neuroscience reveals anything more than we already know from, say, anthropology. For instance, it’s well known that East Asian cultures prize the collective over the individual, and that Americans do the opposite.
Ambady thinks cultural neuroscience does advance understanding. Take the me/mom finding, which, she argues, "attests to the strength of the overlap between self and people close to you in collectivistic cultures and the separation in individualistic cultures. It is important to push the analysis to the level of the brain. " Especially when it shows how fundamental cultural differences are—so fundamental, perhaps, that "universal" notions such as human rights, democracy, and the like may be no such thing. [br] The passage most probably appears in a______.
选项
A、scientific report
B、biography
C、novel
D、newspaper
答案
A
解析
该文章最有可能出现在 。本题为推断题。根据原文的话题,我们可以推断出原文最有可能出现在科学报告中,不可能出现在传记、小说或报刊报道中。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.tihaiku.com/zcyy/3275286.html
相关试题推荐
Theconditionsofartshouldbesimple.Agreatdealmoredependsuponthehe
Theconditionsofartshouldbesimple.Agreatdealmoredependsuponthehe
Whyshouldanyonebuythelatestvolumeintheever-expandingDictionaryofN
Whyshouldanyonebuythelatestvolumeintheever-expandingDictionaryofN
Patents,saidThomasJefferson,shoulddraw"alinebetweenthethingswhich
Patents,saidThomasJefferson,shoulddraw"alinebetweenthethingswhich
Patents,saidThomasJefferson,shoulddraw"alinebetweenthethingswhich
Somepeoplesaythatolderpeopleshouldlivewiththeiradultchildren.Othe
Somepeoplethinkthatthosewhohavereceivedhighereducationshouldbepai
TheHistoryofAmericanIndiansWhenEuropeansdiscovered
随机试题
Aremarkablevarietyofinsectsliveinthisplanet.Morespeciesofinsects
“率真、耿直”反映的个体心理现象是()。A.能力 B.兴趣 C.性格 D.
按照方法不同,经营决策可划分为()。A:程序化决策 B:非程序化决策 C:定
常见的考评尺度主要包括( )。A.量词式的考评标准 B.等级式考评标准 C
2012年6月,我国设立地级三沙市,管辖西沙群岛、中沙群岛、南沙群岛的岛礁及其海
AUC为异亮氨酸的遗传密码,在tRNA异中其相应的反密码应为 A.UA
C
下列含铅烟气中,可用碱液吸收法处理的有( )。A.氧化铅生产含铅烟气 B.蓄
A.脊神经节 B.后角 C.侧角 D.前角 E.脊髓外侧含有交感神经元胞
以下治疗蛛网膜下腔出血的措施不妥的是A.卧床休息4~6周 B.应用止血药物
最新回复
(
0
)