首页
登录
职称英语
Choice blindness: You don’t know what you want[A]We have all
Choice blindness: You don’t know what you want[A]We have all
游客
2024-04-28
35
管理
问题
Choice blindness: You don’t know what you want
[A]We have all heard of experts who fail basic tests of sensory discrimination in their own field: wine snobs(自命不凡的人)who can’t tell red from white wine(though in blackened cups), or art critics who see deep meaning in random lines drawn by a computer. We delight in such stories since anyone claiming to be an authority is fair game. But what if we shine the spotlight on choices we make about everyday things? Experts might be forgiven for being wrong about the limits of their skills as experts, but could we be forgiven for being wrong about the limits of our skills as experts on ourselves?
[B]We have been trying to answer this question using techniques from magic performances. Rather than playing tricks with alternatives presented to participants, we secretly altered the outcomes of their choices, and recorded how they react. For example, in an early study we showed our volunteers pairs of pictures of faces and asked them to choose the most attractive. In some trials, immediately after they made their choice, we asked people to explain the reasons behind their choices.
[C]Unknown to them, we sometimes used a double-card magic trick to secretly exchange one face for the other so they ended up with the face they did not choose. Common sense dictates that all of us would notice such a big change in the outcome of a choice. But the result showed that in 75 per cent of the trials our participants were blind to the mismatch, even offering "reasons" for their "choice".
[D]We called this effect "choice blindness", echoing change blindness, the phenomenon identified by psychologists where a remarkably large number of people fail to spot a major change in their environment. Recall the famous experiments where X asks Y for directions; while Y is struggling to help, X is switched for Z — and Y fails to notice. Researchers are still pondering the full implications, but it does show how little information we use in daily life, and undermines the idea that we know what is going on around us.
[E]When we set out, we aimed to weigh in on the enduring, complicated debate about self-knowledge and intentionality. For all the intimate familiarity we feel we have with decision-making, it is very difficult to know about it from the "inside": one of the great barriers for scientific research is the nature of subjectivity.
[F]As anyone who has ever been in a verbal disagreement can prove, people tend to give elaborate justifications for their decisions, which we have every reason to believe are nothing more than rationalisations(文过饰非)after the event. To prove such people wrong, though, or even provide enough evidence to change their mind, is an entirely different matter: who are you to say what my reasons are?
[G]But with choice blindness we drive a large wedge between intentions and actions in the mind. As our participants give us verbal explanations about choices they never made, we can show them beyond doubt — and prove it — that what they say cannot be true. So our experiments offer a unique window into confabulation(虚构)(the story-telling we do to justify things after the fact)that is otherwise very difficult to come by. We can compare everyday explanations with those under lab conditions, looking for such things as the amount of detail in descriptions, how coherent the narrative is, the emotional tone, or even the timing or flow of the speech. Then we can create a theoretical framework to analyse any kind of exchange.
[H]This framework could provide a clinical use for choice blindness: for example, two of our ongoing studies examine how malingering(装病)might develop into true symptoms, and how confabulation might play a role in obsessive-compulsive disorder(强迫症).
[I]Importantly, the effects of choice blindness go beyond snap judgments. Depending on what our volunteers say in response to the mismatched outcomes of choices(whether they give short or long explanations, give numerical rating or labelling, and so on)we found this interaction could change their future preferences to the extent that they come to prefer the previously rejected alternative. This gives us a rare glimpse into the complicated dynamics of self-feedback("I chose this, I publicly said so, therefore I must like it"), which we suspect lies behind the formation of many everyday preferences.
[J]We also want to explore the boundaries of choice blindness. Of course, it will be limited by choices we know to be of great importance in everyday life. Which bride or bridegroom would fail to notice if someone switched their partner at the altar through amazing sleight of hand(巧妙的手段)? Yet there is ample territory between the absurd idea of spouse-swapping, and the results of our early face experiments.
[K]For example, in one recent study we invited supermarket customers to choose between two paired varieties of jam and tea. In order to switch each participant’s choice without them noticing, we created two sets of "magical" jars, with lids at both ends and a divider inside. The jars looked normal, but were designed to hold one variety of jam or tea at each end, and could easily be flipped over.
[L]Immediately after the participants chose, we asked them to taste their choice again and tell us verbally why they made that choice. Before they did, we turned over the sample containers, so the tasters were given the opposite of what they had intended in their selection. Strikingly, people detected no more than a third of all these trick trials. Even when we switched such remarkably different flavors as spicy cinnamon and apple for bitter grapefruit jam, the participants spotted less than half of all switches.
[M]We have also documented this kind of effect when we simulate online shopping for consumer products such as laptops or cellphones, and even apartments. Our latest tests are exploring moral and political decisions, a domain where reflection and deliberation are supposed to play a central role, but which we believe is perfectly suited to investigating using choice blindness.
[N]Throughout our experiments, as well as registering whether our volunteers noticed that they had been presented with the alternative they did not choose, we also quizzed them about their beliefs about their decision processes. How did they think they would feel if they had been exposed to a study like ours? Did they think they would have noticed the switches? Consistently, between 80 and 90 per cent of people said that they believed they would have noticed something was wrong.
[O]Imagine their surprise, even disbelief, when we told them about the nature of the experiments. In everyday decisionmaking we do see ourselves as knowing a lot about our selves, but like the wine buff or art critic, we often overstate what we know. The good news is that this form of decision snobbery should not be too difficult to treat. Indeed, after reading this article you might already be cured. [br] Researchers suspect that the mechanism of self-feedback is the drive for many everyday preferences.
选项
答案
I
解析
根据题干中的线索词suspect,self-feedback和many everyday preferences将本题出处定位于[I]段末句。该句提到,这给了我们一个难得的机会,使我们得以看到自我反馈(……)的复杂机制,我们猜测,这种自我反馈机制是许多日常偏好的形成原因。题干中的Researchers suspect对应该句的we suspect,the mechanism ofself-feedback是对the complicated dynamics of self-feedback的同义转述,the drive for many everyday preferences对应lies behind the formation of many everyday preferences的同义转述。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.tihaiku.com/zcyy/3575198.html
相关试题推荐
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwantWehaveallh
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwantWehaveallh
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwantWehaveallh
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwantWehaveallh
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwantWehaveallh
ThatwassoseriousamatterthatI______(别无选择,只有报警).hadnochoicebuttocallt
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwant[A]Wehaveall
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwant[A]Wehaveall
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwant[A]Wehaveall
Choiceblindness:Youdon’tknowwhatyouwant[A]Wehaveall
随机试题
Theteacherunabashedlyused(i)______,flatteringhisstudentsinanattemptto
ItwouldbealltooeasytosaythatFacebook’smarketmeltdowniscomingto
下列哪个是迄今为止信息系统开发中应用最普遍、最成熟的方法:A.快速原型法 B.
A.s/3 B.s/7 C.3s D.7s
全面质量管理的全面性表现为()。A.企业全体人员的参与 B.各职能部门的参与
慢性肺源性心脏病心衰时,以下哪项是右心扩大的体征A.肝大 B.双下肢水肿 C
还有半年就要退休的郑老师仍在学校仅凭经验带头上示范课,对徒弟要求严格,以致个别徒
皮格马利翁效应,暗示在本质上,人的情感和观念会不同程度地受到别人下意识的影响,人
下列由全国人民代表大会常务委员会行使职权的有: A.修改宪法 B.选举
下列指标通常被用来衡量风险的是()。A:概率 B:收益率 C:方差 D:相
最新回复
(
0
)