首页
登录
职称英语
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another c
游客
2023-12-13
38
管理
问题
By now, it should come as no surprise when scientists discover yet another case of experience changing the brain. From the sensory information we absorb to the movements we make, our lives leave footprints on the bumps and fissures of our cortex, so much so that experiences can alter "hard-wired" brain structures. Through rehab, stroke patients can coax a region of the motor cortex on the opposite side of the damaged region to pinch-hit, restoring lost mobility; volunteers who are blindfolded for just five days can reprogram their visual cortex to process sound and touch.
Still, scientists have been surprised at how deeply culture—the language we speak, the values we absorb—shapes the brain, and are rethinking findings derived from studies of Westerners. To take one recent example, a region behind the forehead called the medial prefrontal cortex supposedly represents the self: it is active when we ("we" being the Americans in the study) think of our own identity and traits. But with Chinese volunteers, the results were strikingly different. The "me" circuit hummed not only when they thought whether a particular adjective described themselves, but also when they considered whether it described their mother. The Westerners showed no such overlap between self and mom. Depending whether one lives in a culture that views the self as autonomous and unique or as connected to and part of a larger whole, this neural circuit takes on quite different functions.
"Cultural neuroscience," as this new field is called, is about discovering such differences. Some of the findings, as with the "me/mom" circuit, buttress longstanding notions of cultural differences. For instance, it is a cultural cliche that Westerners focus on individual objects while East Asians pay attention to context and background (another manifestation of the individualism-collectivism split). Sure enough, when shown complex, busy scenes, Asian-Americans and non-Asian—Americans recruited different brain regions. The Asians showed more activity in areas that process figure-ground relations—holistic context—while the Americans showed more activity in regions that recognize objects.
Psychologist Nalini Ambady of Tufts found something similar when she and colleagues showed drawings of people in a submissive pose (head down, shoulders hunched) or a dominant one (arms crossed, face forward) to Japanese and Americans. The brain’s dopamine-fueled reward circuit became most active at the sight of the stance—dominant for Americans, submissive for Japanese—that each volunteer’s culture most values, they reported in 2009. This raises an obvious chicken-and-egg question.
Cultural neuroscience wouldn’t be making waves if it found neurobiological bases only for well-known cultural differences. It is also uncovering the unexpected. For instance, a 2006 study found that native Chinese speakers use a different region of the brain to do simple arithmetic (3 + 4) or decide which number is larger than native English speakers do, even though both use Arabic numerals. The Chinese use the circuits that process visual and spatial information and plan movements (the latter may be related to the use of the abacus). But English speakers use language circuits. It is as if the West conceives numbers as just words, but the East imbues them with symbolic, spatial freight. "One would think that neural processes involving basic mathematical computations are universal," says Ambady, but they "seem to be culture-specific. "
Not to be the skunk at this party, but I think it’s important to ask whether neuroscience reveals anything more than we already know from, say, anthropology. For instance, it’s well known that East Asian cultures prize the collective over the individual, and that Americans do the opposite.
Ambady thinks cultural neuroscience does advance understanding. Take the me/mom finding, which, she argues, "attests to the strength of the overlap between self and people close to you in collectivistic cultures and the separation in individualistic cultures. It is important to push the analysis to the level of the brain. " Especially when it shows how fundamental cultural differences are—so fundamental, perhaps, that "universal" notions such as human rights, democracy, and the like may be no such thing. [br] What does "buttress" mean in the third paragraph?
选项
A、contradict
B、doubt
C、are unrelated to
D、support
答案
D
解析
原文第二段中the“me/mom”circuit的发现,表明文化背景和价值观会改变大脑结构。所以第三段中的“buttress”应该指“支持、强化”之意。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.tihaiku.com/zcyy/3269004.html
相关试题推荐
ScientistssaidThursdaythatanewAIDSvaccine,thefirsteverdeclaredto
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
WhenscientistsattheAustralianInstituteofSportrecentlydecidedtoche
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Nooneshouldbeforcedtowearauniformunderanycircumstance.Uniforms
Wemusthavereceivedtheletteryesterday,butitdidn’tarrive.must——shouldmus
随机试题
[originaltext]SouthKorean]aw-makersmountedalash-ditchattempttostopt
[originaltext]M:Excuseme,isthistheaddepartment?W:Sureis.CanIhelpy
二层平面⑥~⑦轴线(含轴线处)需增设的结构框架柱数量为:()
(2021年真题)期权买方只能在期权到期日执行的期权叫做()A.A、认购期
一居民楼内电线的保险丝只能允许同时使用6台空调,现有8户人家各安装了一台空调
“七色光,七色光,太阳的光彩,我们带着七彩梦走向未来。”一束灿烂的阳光照射到下列
某公司从银行贷款,年利率8%,按复利计息,借贷期限5年,每年年末偿还等额本息50
在财产保险合同中,保险人要履行的义务不包括()。A.支付保险赔偿金
某企业生产销售甲、乙两种产品,均采用品种法核算生产成本。2018年12月初开
位置误差是指实际要素的位置对基准的变化量,下列属于位置误差的有( )。A.平行
最新回复
(
0
)