首页
登录
职称英语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilizatio
游客
2023-12-04
31
管理
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through die application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate(灵长目动物). Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis(框架)on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters(支持)the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern" , a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary(初步的)form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate: the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master: a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy—and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions: King, not so much. [br] About religion and morality, Frans de Waal believes that______.
选项
A、religion brings morality to us
B、no code of right and wrong can be discovered out there
C、people who have a religious belief behave appropriately
D、individuals who care about a group can create morality
答案
D
解析
细节题。第八段第二句提到,个体对群体所产生的共鸣与关注有助于形成群体对恰当行为的共识,也就是说道德是我们共同努力的结果,故[D]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.tihaiku.com/zcyy/3242611.html
相关试题推荐
TheRomanticPeriodinAmericanliteraryhistoryendedwiththepublicationofA、
HistoryOfPlymouthPlantationwaswrittenbyA、WilliamBradford.B、CaptainJohn
Throughoutmostofitshistory,______wasamajorproblemthatplaguedIreland.
FrancisBacon’s______isregardedasamilestoneinthehistoryofEnglishpros
TheRomanticPeriodinAmericanLiteraryhistorystartedwiththepublicationof
Historyastheartificialextensionofthesocialmemory(andIwillinglyconce
Themenofhistorywerenotperpetuallylookingintothemirrortomakesureof
BookerT.Washington,aneducationalleader,workedthroughoutthelifetimetoi
Schoolsthroughouttheworldareexperiencingaperiodofrapidchangeand,
Schoolsthroughouttheworldareexperiencingaperiodofrapidchangeand,
随机试题
Aphysicalexaminationisa______tojoiningthearmy.(2002年武汉大学考博试题)A、parliamen
【B1】[br]【B3】[audioFiles]audio_eufm_j51_007(20082)[/audioFiles]rare
TheIELTSExamIELTS:InternationalEnglishLanguage
成人由蛋白质提供能量占膳食总能量适宜的百分比是A.10%~12% B.14%~
包价旅游合同应当采用书面形式,旅行社未与旅游者订立书面合同的,旅游行政主管部门可
以投诉人的数量为依据,旅游投诉的类别主要分为()。A.单独投诉 B.共同投诉
(2020年真题)下列关于金融市场功能的说法,正确的是( )A.金融资产有社会
对培训评估进行可行性评估的内容包括()。A:使管理者知道评估方案已提供并实施
受被测者动机影响不显著的是( )测验。A.成就 B.能力倾向 C.智
工程保险合同的内容较为复杂,工程保险合同谈判常常耗费较多的时间和精力,尤以保险费
最新回复
(
0
)